ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Johan Bengtssona, Erik Olssonb, Jonas Perssona and Robert Bodéna
aDepartment of Medical Sciences, Uppsala University, Akademiska Sjukhuset, Uppsala, Sweden; bDepartment of Women’s and Children’s Health, Uppsala University, Akademiska Sjukhuset, Uppsala, Sweden
Background: The purpose of this study was to investigate whether treatment of a depressive episode with intermittent theta burst stimulation (iTBS) over the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) had any effects on heart rate variability (HRV). We also investigated if changes in HRV covaried with symptom change after iTBS and if HRV could predict symptom change.
Methods: We included 49 patients with a current depressive episode. All were randomized to receive a double-blind treatment course with active or sham iTBS over the DMPFC. HRV data were obtained from 1 h of night data before and after the iTBS. The standard deviation of the RR interval (SDNN) was chosen as primary outcome measure. Depressive, negative, and anxiety symptoms as well as self-rated health were assessed by clinicians or by self-report.
Results: The group×time linear mixed model revealed no effect of iTBS on SDNN (estimate = −1.8, 95% confidence interval [CI]: −19.9 to 16.2). There were neither correlations between HRV and depressive, negative, or anxiety symptom change after iTBS nor with self-assessed health. No predictive value of HRV was found.
Conclusions: Treatment for depression with dorsomedial iTBS had neither negative nor positive effects on the cardiac autonomic nervous system.
KEYWORDS
Depressive disorder; non-invasive brain stimulation; autonomic nervous system; sympathetic; parasympathetic; heart–brain connection
Citation: UPSALA JOURNAL OF MEDICAL SCIENCES 2023, 128, e8949; http://dx.doi.org/10.48101/ujms.v128.8949
Copyright: © 2023 The Author(s). Published by Upsala Medical Society.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Received: 3 August 2022; Revised: 9 November 2022; Accepted: 31 January 2023; Published: 27 March 2023
Competing interests and funding: The authors report no conflict of interest.
This study was funded by unrestricted research grants from the Märta and Nicke Nasvell Foundation, Professor Bror Gadelius Foundation, Selander Foundation, Söderström-Königska Foundation, and Linné Foundation. RB was supported by the Swedish Research Council under Grant 2016-02362, and the Gullstrand Research Fellow grant from Uppsala County Council. JP was supported by The Swedish Brain Foundation. JB was also supported by the Lennander Foundation and granted conference attendance from Anna Maria Lundin’s scholarship committee.
CONTACT Johan Bengtsson johan.bengtsson@neuro.uu.se
Lower levels of heart rate variability (HRV) are generally associated with a wide range of diminished health aspects (1, 2). The interest for HRV in depression stems to a large extent from the association between depression and the risk of cardiovascular disease (3, 4). Unmedicated patients with depression exhibit significantly lower levels of HRV when compared to healthy controls, with effect sizes ranging from –0.096 to –0.462 (5). Medicated patients with depression have even lower HRV, probably partly due to the medications’ anticholinergic properties (6–10). It seems that hereditary factors do not contribute to the low HRV in depression, but psychosocial or life-style factors might explain part of the differences (5, 11, 12). Whether lower HRV mainly constitutes a state or a trait marker in depression is thus still an open question, and another way of approaching this issue has been to investigate if changes in depressive symptoms are accompanied by changes in HRV. For pharmacological or psychotherapeutic interventions in depression, it appears that symptom change is unrelated to changes in HRV, even if there are inconsistencies in the literature (8, 13). These findings favor the role of HRV as a trait marker in depression. Initiation or cessation of antidepressant pharmacological treatment has, however, been shown to decrease or increase HRV, respectively – further pointing out the important role of medication effects (10). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) (14, 15) is a non-pharmacological treatment option for depression and might therefore provide an opportunity to study depressive symptom change and HRV without confounding medication effects. Various rTMS treatment protocols are being used, of which intermittent theta burst stimulation (iTBS) (16) has been of special interest since it can reach the same efficacy with a shorter duration of the daily treatment protocol (17). The most commonly used treatment target for rTMS in depression is the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), which is part of the central autonomic modulation network, also including the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), insula, amygdala, and other structures (18). The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is another area implicated in central autonomic modulation, and it is located just beneath the DMPFC (19). Using angled magnetic stimulation coils allows for modulating the ACC when applied over the DMPFC (20). It could therefore be expected that stimulation of both the DLPFC and the DMPFC would affect cardiac autonomic regulation. Indeed, while rTMS seems to increase HRV during the actual stimulation in healthy individuals (21–24), there is a scarcity of studies on the cardiac autonomic effects of rTMS in depression following a full treatment course. An open study reported increased HRV after rTMS (25) but the only sham-controlled study measuring the concomitant effect of iTBS found no enduring effects on HRV, albeit acute changes during the iTBS session, thus converging with the findings in healthy individuals (26). Broadening the diagnostic perspective to schizophrenia, a recent large, multi-center and sham-controlled study found no effects of an rTMS treatment course on heart rate (27). Regarding HRV and concomitant symptom change in depression after rTMS, there are only few reports of such associations. The open study mentioned above reported a correlation between post-treatment depression scores and post-treatment HRV, but there were no comparisons with baseline values (25). Another study reported a non-significant correlation between heart rate decelerations during the first treatment session and greater symptom reduction (26). This finding has been argued to open up for heart rate decelerations as a predictor for treatment response in depression (28), but to the best of our knowledge, there are no studies on HRV as a predictor for treatment response after rTMS. The results of HRV as a predictor of treatment response for other treatment options in depression have hitherto been mixed (29–31).
In summary, there is only one open study reporting an increase in HRV after a full treatment session of rTMS. This finding would be strengthened if replicated with a sham-controlled design. Also, since other treatment options for depression, such as tricyclic antidepressant agents (TCAs), clearly diminish HRV, a lack of such negative treatment side effects of rTMS would be beneficial. The present study aimed at investigating the effect on HRV of a treatment course with iTBS, targeting the DMPFC in patients with depression. Further aims were to investigate the correlation between symptom change and change in HRV and also to investigate whether baseline HRV would predict symptom change.
The 49 participants were a subsample of patients from a randomized controlled trial (clinicaltrials.gov identification number NCT02905604) of dorsomedial prefrontal iTBS for negative symptoms in schizophrenia or depression, and from an add-on brain-imaging study (32, 33). The transdiagnostic approach was based on the observed overlap between negative and depressive symptoms (34). Inclusion criteria were originally designed for the above-mentioned trial, but for the current study, they were as follows: written informed consent, being 18–59 years old, a diagnosis of uni- or bipolar depression established by a psychiatrist in clinical routine care and verified through a Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I., Swedish translation of version 6.0.0) (35), less or equal than 40 points on The Motivation and Pleasure Scale-Self-Report (MAP-SR) (36), and no changes in medication during the past month. Exclusion criteria were standard rTMS such as metals implanted in the head, epilepsy, pacemakers, vagus nerve stimulators, medication pumps, etc. (37). Other exclusion criteria were any condition implicating a substantial risk of non-compliance or loss of follow-up, active substance use disorder (except nicotine and caffeine), and pregnancy. The patients with schizophrenia recruited to the main trial were excluded from the current study (n = 16). See Figure 1 for a flow chart. The study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki declaration and approved by the Research Ethical Review Board in Uppsala. The trial was conducted between 2016 and 2020 at Uppsala university hospital in Sweden.
Figure 1. CONSORT flow chart of the subsample constituting the current study.
The DMPFC was localized with neuronavigation (33) or defined as 25% of the distance between the nasion and the inion (n = 11). The iTBS was delivered with a magnetic stimulator Magpro X100. The coil was an angled combined active/placebo coil (Cool-DB80 A/P) with two identical sides, of which one side was shielded, so that the majority of the magnetic field did not reach the participant. Upon entering a randomization code into the stimulator, the operator was instructed by the software which side to position toward the participant’s head (tangentially to the scalp, handle toward the right side of the participant). Resting motor threshold was defined as the lowest magnetic stimulator output needed for a visually observable muscle contraction in the musculus extensor hallucis longus in the foot in 50% of the trials, determined by an automated maximum likelihood strategy (38, 39). The active iTBS was a modified version of earlier protocols (39, 40), with 20 trains of stimulation with right-left stimulation and 20 trains with left-right stimulation. A second identical treatment session was delivered after a 15 min break (41, 42). Stimulation was applied at 90% of resting motor threshold with a total of 2,400 pulses per day. The sham iTBS comprised an identical protocol but with the shielded side of the coil toward the participant, whereby only a weak magnetic field was applied. In addition, two transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) electrodes were placed under the coil of both the active and sham participants’ foreheads. In order to also mimic the sensation of the magnetic stimulation in the sham group, a current of maximum 4 mA (scaled to the magnetic stimulator output intensity) was delivered synchronous with the magnetic pulses. The treatment was delivered by a research nurse, and the symptom raters were not present during the treatment sessions. Stimulation intensity was ramped up to minimize discomfort, aiming at 10 consecutive weekdays of treatment at target intensity (32).
The two-electrode heart rate recorder Firstbeat Bodyguard 2 (Firstbeat Technologies Ltd., Jyväskylä, Finland) was used to monitor heart rate. The device was attached across the chest by a research nurse on the morning the day before the first treatment day. After this visit, the participants wore the device until the next day, also during the night, and returned it when they came back for their first treatment session. The same procedure was repeated the day after the last treatment, 2 weeks after baseline. Sampling rate was 1,000 Hz with a resolution of 1 ms. Since the aim of this study was to assess the potential lasting effects on HRV, an hour of data were extracted during the night (midnight to 5 am). This hour was chosen from a period where heart rate seemed stable, when inspected visually. The chosen hour varied between subjects depending on visual inspection of where there were fewest artifacts. This hour could also vary between each participant’s baseline and follow-up data, but was aimed to be the same. Within the chosen time period, the following HRV metrics were calculated: the standard deviation of the RR interval (SDNN), the root mean square of successive RR interval differences (RMSSD), high frequency HRV (HF), low frequency HRV (LF), LF/HF ratio, and the RR triangular index (2). LF and HF were logaritmized. As an open study has previously reported changes in SDNN after rTMS, this metric was chosen as the primary outcome measure to enable comparisons (25). If artifact correction was necessary after visual inspection, the inbuilt algorithm in the software Kubios HRV Standard (version 3.0.2) was used. The algorithm compares the length of each interbeat interval (IBI) to an average of the surrounding IBIs and classifies IBIs that differ from a selected threshold as potential artifacts.
In a subset of patients (n = 37), we also extracted HRV data from a 5-min daytime period when the participants were resting, sitting in a supine position just after the device was attached. This data collection was performed both the day before the first treatment session (baseline) and 4 weeks after baseline. From this period, we extracted HF-HRV data. These data were used for a separate analysis of the lasting effects of iTBS on HRV. The data were only available in a subset of patients due to slightly different designs of the scheduled time frames of the main trial (32) and the add-on brain imaging study (33). This was done in order to evaluate the effect of iTBS on two different standard HRV metrics – SDNN and HF-HRV.
Since the original trial aimed at treating negative symptoms with a transdiagnostic approach, patients were assessed at the baseline visit (the day before the first treatment day) by a trained physician with the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS) (43, 44) and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale – extended (BPRS-E) (45). Participants also filled in self-reports: sociodemographic data, nicotine consumption, The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (46), The Drug Use Disorders Identification Test (DUDIT) (47), Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale – Self-report (48), and self-assessed health with the EQ-VAS (49). The same interviews and symptom ratings were repeated the day after the last treatment session (i.e. 2 weeks from baseline), and also 4 weeks after baseline. After that, the concealed treatment allocation was unblinded, and participants who had been receiving sham iTBS were offered an open treatment course.
All data were assessed for normality by visual inspection of histograms, Q-Q plots, and with the Shapiro–Wilk test. Means and standard deviations were reported if variables were found to be normally distributed and medians and inter-quartile ranges if the data distributions were assessed to be skewed. To investigate differences in demographic variables between the groups, we used the independent-samples t-test for continuous normally distributed variables, Mann–Whitney U-test for continuous non-normally distributed variables, and chi square or Fisher’s exact test for dichotomous and rank variables.
In the primary analysis, we used a linear mixed model using maximum likelihood estimation with random intercept per subject to assess group and time effects of iTBS on SDNN (or HF-HRV in the subsample of 37 patients).
For the correlation between HRV metrics and symptom change, Pearson correlation coefficients were computed to assess correlations between delta values of symptoms and delta values of SDNN. The symptom ratings from the assessment 2 weeks after baseline were used, except for eight participants, where the ratings 4 weeks after baseline were used (due to slightly different time frames for the participants also participating in the brain imaging study mentioned above).
To assess the predictive value of baseline SDNN on symptom change, we used a linear regression model with delta values of symptoms as the dependent variable and baseline SDNN as the independent variable. Here, we only analyzed the active iTBS data from both the 25 participants receiving active iTBS at first and the data from the participants from the sham group who decided to enter an open phase with active iTBS (n = 17), resulting in a sample of 42 participants. The procedures for the iTBS sessions, HRV assessments, and symptom ratings were identical for both the active and sham groups. The symptom ratings from the assessment 2 weeks after baseline were used, except for six participants, where the ratings 4 weeks after baseline were used (due to the same reason as mentioned above). We assessed linearity between the variables through visual inspection. The presence of influential outliers was also inspected visually as well as with the Cook’s Distance. Multicollinearity was assessed with the variance inflation factor. The Durbin–Watson statistic was used to test the independency of the residuals’ values, and PP plots were used to inspect the distribution of the residuals. The residuals’ variance indicated no severe heteroscedasticity. SPSS version 26 was used for the statistical calculations.
All demographic data are presented in Table 1. Overall, the two randomized groups were similar, but there were more patients in the sham group with a significant difference regarding body mass index (BMI) and prescription of positive chronotropic drugs. However, there were no significant differences in baseline SDNN between the groups (independent t-test, t = 0.29, P = 0.774). For BMI, a linear regression model revealed no significant impact of BMI on baseline SDNN (β = 0.016, P = 0.914). There was one participant in the active group on antidiabetic treatment.
Variable | Active (n = 25) | Sham (n = 24) | Pa |
Age (years), median (IQR) | 27 (14) | 27 (12) | 0.674 |
Sex, n (female/male) | 14/11 | 14/10 | 0.869 |
BMI (kg/m2), median (IQR) | 25 (12) | 23 (7) | 0.040 |
Bipolar depression | 1 (4) | 3 (13) | 0.349 |
Comorbidity anxiety disorders | 9 (36) | 11 (46) | 0.484 |
Comorbidity neuropsychiatric disorders | 6 (24) | 8 (33) | 0.470 |
Supported housing | 2 (8) | 5 (21) | 0.247 |
Level of education | |||
Primary school | 3 (12) | 5 (21) | 0.463 |
High school | 12 (48) | 15 (63) | 0.308 |
Higher education | 10 (40) | 3 (13) | 0.051 |
In current employment or studying | 15 (60) | 15 (63) | 0.858 |
Sheehan Disability Scale, median (IQR) | 19 (10) | 18 (11) | 0.865 |
Nicotine useb | 7 (28) | 9 (38) | 0.478 |
AUDIT total score, median (IQR) | 3 (3) | 5 (6) | 0.643 |
DUDIT total score, median (IQR) | 0 (8) | 0 (3) | 0.940 |
PSQI sleeping time (h), mean (SD)c | 8 (2) | 7 (2) | 0.096 |
Baseline HRV data starting time, mean time a.m. (SD in min) | 2:28 (68) | 2:39 (58) | 0.529 |
Follow-up HRV data starting time, mean time a.m. (SD in min) | 2:38 (71) | 2:42 (67) | 0.701 |
Difference between starting times (baseline – follow-up HRV data), min, mean (SD) | 22 (29) | 27 (41) | 0.611 |
Prescriptions | |||
SSRI | 6 (24) | 5 (21) | 0.791 |
SNRI | 7 (28) | 9 (38) | 0.478 |
TCA | 5 (20) | 2 (8) | 0.417 |
Antidepressant combination | 10 (40) | 7 (29) | 0.426 |
Lithium | 6 (24) | 4 (17) | 0.725 |
Antipsychotic | 4 (16) | 5 (21) | 0.725 |
Positive chronotropic drugd | 2 (8) | 9 (38) | 0.018 |
Negative chronotropic drugd | 1 (4) | 4 (17) | 0.189 |
IQR: interquartile range; BMI: body mass index; SD: standard deviation; AUDIT: Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test; DUDIT: Drug Use Disorders Identification Test; PSQI: Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; SSRI: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor; SNRI: Serotonin and Norepinephrine Reuptake Inhibitors; TCA: tricyclic antidepressant agent; HRV: heart rate variability. | |||
aIndependent-samples t-test/Mann–Whitney U-test for continuous variables, chi square for dichotomic variables, and Fisher’s exact test if n < 5 in any cell with dichotomic variables. | |||
bTobacco or Swedish snuff. | |||
cTwo missing. | |||
dPositive chronotropic drugs included dexamphetamine, levothyroxine, and methylphenidate. Negative chronotropic drugs included betablockers, guanfacine, and thiamazole. |
See Table 2 for HRV and symptom burden data. The sham group had lower heart rate both at baseline and at follow-up. The difference was borderline significant at baseline (independent sample t-test, P = 0.052) and significant at follow-up (P = 0.028). There were no significant differences in SDNN between the groups, neither at baseline nor at follow-up.
Regarding the effect of iTBS on SDNN, there was no interaction effect of group×time (estimate = −1.8, 95% confidence interval [CI]: −19.9 to 16.2). No effects on the secondary HRV variables were noted.
There were no significant symptom reduction and no correlations between change in SDNN and symptom change on CAINS, MADRS-S, BPRS anxiety subscale, or self-assessed health. In the correlation analyses, we analyzed the active and sham groups together, but subgroup analyses of the active and sham group separately revealed no differences (data not shown). See Figure 2 for a graphical presentation of the correlations.
Figure 2. Scatter plots for delta values of symptoms and delta values of HRV. Note that a positive value of EQ-VAS indicates health improvement, whereas positive values on the other scales represent increased symptom load.
r = Pearson correlation coefficient for the regression line (similar results using Spearman’s correlation coefficients).
HRV: heart rate variability; SDNN: standard deviation of the RR interval; CAINS: Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms; MADRS-S: Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale self-rating; BPRS: Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; EQ-VAS: EuroQoL Group Visual Analogue Scale.
aSum of items Anxiety and Tension.
Regarding the predictive value of SDNN, there was no effect of baseline values of SDNN on any of the symptom categories, see Table 3.
Variable | SDNN | ||
B | beta | P | |
CAINS | 0.061 | 0.146 | 0.355 |
MADRS-S | −0.022 | −0.055 | 0.732 |
BPRS anxietya | 0.004 | 0.031 | 0.843 |
EQ-VAS | 0.063 | 0.078 | 0.621 |
Linear regression results. Dependent variable: delta values of symptoms (n = 42). | |||
HRV: heart rate variability; iTBS: intermittent theta burst stimulation; SDNN: standard deviation of the RR interval; CAINS: Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms; MADRS-S: Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale self-rating; BPRS: Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; EQ-VAS: EuroQoL Group Visual Analogue Scale. | |||
aSum of items Anxiety and Tension. |
The main analysis of the effect of iTBS on HRV was also run in the subsample of patients (n = 37) with 5 min daytime resting HRV data available (before the treatment course and 4 weeks after baseline). In this model, we used logarithmic HF-HRV as the dependent variable. The results did not differ substantially from the main analysis (estimate = −0.24, 95% CI: −1.60 to 1.12).
To the best of our knowledge, this is so far the largest study investigating the effect on HRV after a treatment course of iTBS targeting the DMPFC in depression. We did not detect any significant effects on the HRV metrics. Neither there were any correlations between change in HRV and symptom change detected, nor there was any predictive value of baseline HRV on symptom change.
First, it has to be pointed out that we specifically assessed HRV before and after the iTBS treatment course and therefore cannot draw any conclusions about whether iTBS has an acute and transient effect on HRV. Comparisons with studies on healthy individuals are hindered due to the fact that no healthy individuals receive longer treatment courses. Of the sham-controlled studies on healthy individuals, there have been no effects that lasted over time (22, 50–54). Regarding depression, the only earlier sham-controlled study investigated the acute effects of iTBS in 15 patients with depression and mainly focused on heart rate decelerations, which complicates comparisons with our investigation of the enduring effects of a treatment series with iTBS (26). Concerning HRV in that study, it was found that HRV was significantly more affected by active iTBS than sham. The sham condition though (shielded coil placed over the vertex) did not produce any comparable pain stimuli as the active iTBS, which leaves the possibility to open that these observations are connected to the pain effects of the stimuli, since arousal effects have been shown to be important (55). The treatment target in the same study was the DLPFC and not the DMPFC as in our study. Even if both of these structures are connected to the ACC, which is an important component of the central autonomic network (18, 19), it cannot be ruled out that differential locations implicate separate effects on HRV. Indeed, a study from the same research group indicates this (56). Nevertheless, taken together, these differences hamper further comparisons between the two studies.
As for non-sham-controlled studies of the effect of rTMS on HRV, an open study found increased SDNN between baseline and follow-up 2 weeks later in 30 patients with depression treated with rTMS (targeting DLPFC using 5-cm-rule, 15 Hz, 100% of resting motor threshold (rMT), 1,500 pulses per session). They assessed HRV before and after the treatment, which possibly eliminates the potential confounding effects of the simultaneous pain or sensational stimuli of rTMS. However, since the study was not sham-controlled, there can be no speculations of the specific effects of rTMS on HRV (25). In our study, we did not observe any changes at all, regardless of active or sham treatment. However, our studies differ in many aspects. The sample in the study by Udupa et al. consisted of drug-naïve patients without psychiatric or somatic comorbidities and with a milder depression score rating at baseline, whereas our sample consisted of patients on medication, with comorbidities and higher baseline depression rating scores. The HRV assessments also differ in that we used an hour of night data, whereas their study used 5 min of resting data as well as different assessment conditions. Comparisons between the studies must therefore be cautious.
Regarding the correlation between HRV change and symptom change, we did not find any such correlations. In the literature, this question has been of interest as a way of approaching the causal interactions between HRV and symptom change. In an open study of 27 patients receiving rTMS, there was no correlation between symptom reduction and HRV or other autonomic parameters (57). Adding three patients to the same sample yielded a correlation between follow-up values of LF/HF and depressive symptoms at follow-up, but there were no comparisons with baseline values (25).
We could not find any predictive value of HRV regarding treatment response. Here, we analyzed all open treatment sessions in our study (n = 42). Earlier efforts of using HRV as a predictor of treatment response in depression have not led to any conclusive results, even if there are positive findings of other treatment modalities scattered in the literature (29, 30). Results from the only sham-controlled rTMS study investigating this issue in 15 patients with depression could not find any correlations between decelerations of heart rate early in the treatment course and clinical response after the treatment, even if there was a trend toward such an effect (26). More research is needed before HRV can achieve acceptable prediction properties in the field of depression treatment.
There are some limitations of our study. Even though our sample is, to our knowledge, the so far largest sample investigating the effects of a sham controlled treatment course of iTBS on HRV, it is still a small sample size. It has been argued that interpreting CIs is better than post-hoc analyses to judge the robustness of ‘null’ results (58). Our results yielded quite wide CIs for all the outcome measures (see Table 2). The CIs for SDNN spanned from a decrease by approximately 20 ms to an increase by 16 ms. It can therefore be stated that iTBS at least does not affect SDNN more or less than that.
Regarding the HRV data extraction, we could not ascertain that the participants actually slept. However, the time frame of midnight to 5 am is a time when most people do sleep, and the participants in both groups reported a sleeping time of around 7 h. Furthermore, the mean starting time for the HRV data did not differ between the groups. The baseline and the follow-up HRV data were in some cases extracted from different times during the night, but the mean difference was only 25 min in the whole sample (see Table 1). However, HRV might also fluctuate according to sleep phase, which we were unable to assess (59). This might have introduced uncontrolled variability and thus be a source for our negative findings. Our converging results from the subsample analysis of a standard 5 min HRV recording however strengthen our results somewhat. Future studies assessing HRV during night would merit from a more detailed sleep phase assessment.
We chose SDNN as our primary outcome. Our main interest has not been the parasympathetic nervous system in isolation, and SDNN originates from both sympathetic and parasympathetic signaling (60). SDNN is encouraged for use in longer recordings, even if a 24-h recording would have been preferable. Furthermore, it has the advantage of being more easily interpreted and is also used in the important prediction study of HRV and cardiovascular risk (61). In the main analysis, we also analyzed a subsample (n = 37) using the vagal measure of HF-HRV, and the results did not differ to that of SDNN.
Some argue that baseline statistical comparisons between the groups in randomized data are not necessary (62). However, if the study sample is restricted, there is still a risk of imbalance between the groups. In the current study, there were no significant differences between the treatment groups regarding known baseline factors that might affect the outcome. We therefore refrained from controlling for factors such as age and sex.
The participants’ medications were quite diverse, possibly reflecting the heterogeneity of the treatment-resistant depression concept and the aim of recruiting a clinically representative sample. Importantly, all drug regimens were kept stable during this study, and even though these medications could have an impact on HRV, it is therefore unlikely that they would contribute to a change in HRV during the study period. Some drugs prescribed (most notably TCAs) could cause a high occupancy of the muscarinic receptors in the heart and might thereby diminish the vagal possibilities to modulate heart rate, thus rendering these participants unable to change their HRV. There were however no substantial differences in TCA prescription between the active and the sham groups.
Eight participants’ symptom ratings originated from 4 weeks after baseline rather than 2 weeks after baseline. However, at least some of the symptoms assessed are not subject to rapid fluctuations but rather quite stable. Furthermore, excluding these participants did not materially change the results (data not shown).
The use of delta values in correlational analyses may introduce measurement error from both baseline and follow-up assessments with subsequent unreliability. Our results from these analyses are therefore to be interpreted with caution, and the reader is referred to Figure 1 for a visual interpretation.
Our sample consisted of patients recruited to a transdiagnostic study of the effects of iTBS on negative symptoms. The results are therefore not necessarily generalizable to a wider group of patients with depression. The patients who come in contact with rTMS are however often labeled as ‘treatment resistant’, and even if these patients can be defined in many ways (63, 64), our sample is probably comparable to this group. Some of these patients are already exposed to treatments with well documented effects on their cardiac autonomic nervous system functioning, for example, TCAs (6). Our results indicate that rTMS lacks such effects.
We conclude that treatment with dorsomedial iTBS did not increase HRV, but it did not have any negative effects on the cardiac autonomic nervous system.
We wish to acknowledge statistician Hans Arinell for help with the statistical analyses.
Johan Bengtsson, Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – Original Draft, Visualization.
Erik Olsson, Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – Review & Editing.
Jonas Persson, Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – Review & Editing.
Robert Bodén, Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Writing – Review & Editing, Supervision.
Johan Bengtsson https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6043-0761
Erik Olsson https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1591-7407
Jonas Persson https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2162-0949
Robert Bodén https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2198-8842
1. | Berntson GG, Bigger JT, Jr., Eckberg DL, Grossman P, Kaufmann PG, Malik M, et al. Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology. 1997;34:623–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x |
2. | Malik M. Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Eur Heart J. 1996;17:354–81. |
3. | Carney RM, Freedland KE. Depression and coronary heart disease. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2017;14:145–55. doi: 10.1038/nrcardio.2016.181 |
4. | Duivis HE, de Jonge P, Penninx BW, Na BY, Cohen BE, Whooley MA. Depressive symptoms, health behaviors, and subsequent inflammation in patients with coronary heart disease: prospective findings from the heart and soul study. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:913–20. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10081163 |
5. | Koch C, Wilhelm M, Salzmann S, Rief W, Euteneuer F. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability in major depression. Psychol Med. 2019;49: 1948–57. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001351 |
6. | Alvares GA, Quintana DS, Hickie IB, Guastella AJ. Autonomic nervous system dysfunction in psychiatric disorders and the impact of psychotropic medications: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2016;41:89–104. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140217 |
7. | Bengtsson J, Olsson E, Igelstrom H, Persson J, Boden R. Ambulatory heart rate variability in Schizophrenia or depression: impact of anticholinergic burden and other factors. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2021;41:121–8. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0000000000001356 |
8. | Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Gray MA, Felmingham KL, Brown K, Gatt JM. Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: a review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:1067–74. doi: doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012 |
9. | Licht CM, de Geus EJ, Zitman FG, Hoogendijk WJ, van Dyck R, Penninx BW. Association between major depressive disorder and heart rate variability in the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:1358–67. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.12.1358 |
10. | Licht CM, de Geus EJ, van Dyck R, Penninx BW. Longitudinal evidence for unfavorable effects of antidepressants on heart rate variability. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:861–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.032 |
11. | Berger S, Schulz S, Kletta C, Voss A, Bar KJ. Autonomic modulation in healthy first-degree relatives of patients with major depressive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:1723–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.05.018 |
12. | Shah AS, Alonso A, Whitsel EA, Soliman EZ, Vaccarino V, Shah AJ. Association of psychosocial factors with short-term resting heart rate variability: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. J Am Heart Assoc. 2021;10:e017172. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.120.017172 |
13. | Neyer S, Witthoft M, Cropley M, Pawelzik M, Lugo RG, Sutterlin S. Reduction of depressive symptoms during inpatient treatment is not associated with changes in heart rate variability. PLoS One. 2021;16:e0248686. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248686 |
14. | Lefaucheur JP, Aleman A, Baeken C, Benninger DH, Brunelin J, Di Lazzaro V, et al. Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): an update (2014–2018). Clin Neurophysiol. 2020;131:474–528. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.11.002 |
15. | Rossi S, Antal A, Bestmann S, Bikson M, Brewer C, Brockmoller J, et al. Safety and recommendations for TMS use in healthy subjects and patient populations, with updates on training, ethical and regulatory issues: expert guidelines. Clin Neurophysiol. 2021;132:269–306. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.10.003 |
16. | Huang YZ, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC. Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron. 2005;45:201–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033 |
17. | Blumberger DM, Vila-Rodriguez F, Thorpe KE, Feffer K, Noda Y, Giacobbe P, et al. Effectiveness of theta burst versus high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with depression (THREE-D): a randomised non-inferiority trial. Lancet. 2018;391:1683–92. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30295-2 |
18. | Palma JA, Benarroch EE. Neural control of the heart: recent concepts and clinical correlations. Neurology. 2014;83:261–71. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000605 |
19. | Thayer JF, Ahs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ, 3rd, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:747–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009 |
20. | Hayward G, Mehta MA, Harmer C, Spinks TJ, Grasby PM, Goodwin GM. Exploring the physiological effects of double-cone coil TMS over the medial frontal cortex on the anterior cingulate cortex: an H2(15)O PET study. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:2224–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05430.x |
21. | Makovac E, Thayer JF, Ottaviani C. A meta-analysis of non-invasive brain stimulation and autonomic functioning: implications for brain-heart pathways to cardiovascular disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;74:330–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.001 |
22. | Remue J, Vanderhasselt MA, Baeken C, Rossi V, Tullo J, De Raedt R. The effect of a single HF-rTMS session over the left DLPFC on the physiological stress response as measured by heart rate variability. Neuropsychology. 2016;30:756–66. doi: 10.1037/neu0000255 |
23. | Rossi S, Santarnecchi E, Valenza G, Ulivelli M. The heart side of brain neuromodulation. Philos Trans A Math Phys Eng Sci. 2016;374. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2015.0187 |
24. | Schestatsky P, Simis M, Freeman R, Pascual-Leone A, Fregni F. Non-invasive brain stimulation and the autonomic nervous system. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124:1716–28. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.03.020 |
25. | Udupa K, Thirthalli J, Sathyaprabha TN, Kishore KR, Raju TR, Gangadhar BN. Differential actions of antidepressant treatments on cardiac autonomic alterations in depression: a prospective comparison. Asian J Psychiatr. 2011;4:100–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2011.02.006 |
26. | Iseger TA, Arns M, Downar J, Blumberger DM, Daskalakis ZJ, Vila-Rodriguez F. Cardiovascular differences between sham and active iTBS related to treatment response in MDD. Brain Stimul. 2020;13:167–74. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.09.016 |
27. | Campana M, Wagner E, Wobrock T, Langguth B, Landgrebe M, Eichhammer P, et al. Effects of high-frequency prefrontal rTMS on heart frequency rates and blood pressure in schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;140:243–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.06.010 |
28. | Michael JA, Kaur M. The heart-brain connection in depression: can it inform a personalised approach for repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) treatment? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;127: 136–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.016 |
29. | Choi KW, Jeon HJ. Heart rate variability for the prediction of treatment response in major depressive disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:607. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00607 |
30. | Meyer T, Brunovsky M, Horacek J, Novak T, Andrashko V, Seifritz E, et al. Predictive value of heart rate in treatment of major depression with ketamine in two controlled trials. Clin Neurophysiol. 2021;132:1339–46. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2021.01.030 |
31. | Noda Y, Knyahnytska Y, Zomorrodi R, Downar J, Rajji TK, Daskalakis ZJ, et al. Vagally mediated heart rate variability is associated with executive function changes in patients with treatment-resistant depression following magnetic seizure therapy. Neuromodulation. 2020;25:1378–86. doi: 10.1111/ner.13262 |
32. | Boden R, Bengtsson J, Thornblom E, Struckmann W, Persson J. Dorsomedial prefrontal theta burst stimulation to treat anhedonia, avolition, and blunted affect in schizophrenia or depression – a randomized controlled trial. J Affect Disord. 2021;290:308–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.04.053 |
33. | Persson J, Struckmann W, Gingnell M, Fallmar D, Boden R. Intermittent theta burst stimulation over the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex modulates resting-state connectivity in depressive patients: a sham-controlled study. Behav Brain Res. 2020;394:112834. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112834 |
34. | Richter J, Holz L, Hesse K, Wildgruber D, Klingberg S. Measurement of negative and depressive symptoms: discriminatory relevance of affect and expression. Eur Psychiatry. 2019;55:23–8. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.09.008 |
35. | Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-http://dx.doi.org/10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59 Suppl 20:22–33;quiz 4–57. |
36. | Llerena K, Park SG, McCarthy JM, Couture SM, Bennett ME, Blanchard JJ. The Motivation and Pleasure Scale-Self-Report (MAP-SR): reliability and validity of a self-report measure of negative symptoms. Compr Psychiatry. 2013;54:568–74. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.12.001 |
37. | Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A. Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009;120:2008–39. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016 |
38. | Ah Sen CB, Fassett HJ, El-Sayes J, Turco CV, Hameer MM, Nelson AJ. Active and resting motor threshold are efficiently obtained with adaptive threshold hunting. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0186007. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0186007 |
39. | Bakker N, Shahab S, Giacobbe P, Blumberger DM, Daskalakis ZJ, Kennedy SH, et al. rTMS of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex for major depression: safety, tolerability, effectiveness, and outcome predictors for 10 Hz versus intermittent theta-burst stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:208–15. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.11.002 |
40. | Chistyakov AV, Rubicsek O, Kaplan B, Zaaroor M, Klein E. Safety, tolerability and preliminary evidence for antidepressant efficacy of theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with major depression. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;13:387–93. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710000027 |
41. | Duprat R, Desmyter S, Rudi DR, van Heeringen K, Van den Abbeele D, Tandt H, et al. Accelerated intermittent theta burst stimulation treatment in medication-resistant major depression: a fast road to remission? J Affect Disord. 2016;200:6–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.015 |
42. | Tse NY, Goldsworthy MR, Ridding MC, Coxon JP, Fitzgerald PB, Fornito A, et al. The effect of stimulation interval on plasticity following repeated blocks of intermittent theta burst stimulation. Sci Rep. 2018;8:8526. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26791-w |
43. | Bengtsson J, Boden R, Neider D, Cernvall M. A blinded validation of the Swedish version of the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS). Nord J Psychiatry. 2022;76:44–51. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2021.1933174 |
44. | Kring AM, Gur RE, Blanchard JJ, Horan WP, Reise SP. The Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS): final development and validation. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170:165–72. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010109 |
45. | Zanello A, Berthoud L, Ventura J, Merlo MC. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (version 4.0) factorial structure and its sensitivity in the treatment of outpatients with unipolar depression. Psychiatry Res. 2013;210:626–33. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.07.001 |
46. | Bohn MJ, Babor TF, Kranzler HR. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): validation of a screening instrument for use in medical settings. J Stud Alcohol. 1995;56:423–32. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.423 |
47. | Berman AH, Bergman H, Palmstierna T, Schlyter F. Evaluation of the Drug Use Disorders Identification Test (DUDIT) in criminal justice and detoxification settings and in a Swedish population sample. Eur Addict Res. 2005;11:22–31. doi: 10.1159/000081413 |
48. | Svanborg P, Asberg M. A comparison between the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and the self-rating version of the Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS). J Affect Disord. 2001;64:203–16. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00242-1 |
49. | Burstrom K, Sun S, Gerdtham UG, Henriksson M, Johannesson M, Levin LA, et al. Swedish experience-based value sets for EQ-5D health states. Qual Life Res. 2014;23:431–42. doi: 10.1007/s11136-013-0496-4 |
50. | Era V, Carnevali L, Thayer JF, Candidi M, Ottaviani C. Dissociating cognitive, behavioral and physiological stress-related responses through dorsolateral prefrontal cortex inhibition. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2021;124:105070. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.105070 |
51. | Foerster A, Schmitz JM, Nouri S, Claus D. Safety of rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation: heart rate and blood pressure changes. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1997;104:207–12. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(97)00016-6 |
52. | Pulopulos MM, Schmausser M, De Smet S, Vanderhasselt MA, Baliyan S, Venero C, et al. The effect of HF-rTMS over the left DLPFC on stress regulation as measured by cortisol and heart rate variability. Horm Behav. 2020;124:104803. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104803 |
53. | Vernieri F, Altamura C, Palazzo P, Altavilla R, Fabrizio E, Fini R, et al. 1-Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation increases cerebral vasomotor reactivity: a possible autonomic nervous system modulation. Brain Stimul. 2014;7:281–6. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.12.014 |
54. | Yoshida T, Yoshino A, Kobayashi Y, Inoue M, Kamakura K, Nomura S. Effects of slow repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on heart rate variability according to power spectrum analysis. J Neurol Sci. 2001;184:77–80. doi: 10.1016/s0022-510x(00)00505-0 |
55. | Poppa T, de Witte S, Vanderhasselt MA, Bechara A, Baeken C. Theta-burst stimulation and frontotemporal regulation of cardiovascular autonomic outputs: the role of state anxiety. Int J Psychophysiol. 2020;149:25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.12.011 |
56. | Iseger TA, Padberg F, Kenemans JL, van Dijk H, Arns M. Neuro-Cardiac-Guided TMS (NCG TMS): a replication and extension study. Biol Psychol. 2021;162:108097. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108097 |
57. | Udupa K, Sathyaprabha TN, Thirthalli J, Kishore KR, Raju TR, Gangadhar BN. Modulation of cardiac autonomic functions in patients with major depression treated with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Affect Disord. 2007;104:231–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.04.002 |
58. | Levine M, Ensom MH. Post hoc power analysis: an idea whose time has passed? Pharmacotherapy. 2001;21:405–9. doi: 10.1592/phco.21.5.405.34503 |
59. | Stein PK, Pu Y. Heart rate variability, sleep and sleep disorders. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16:47–66. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.02.005 |
60. | Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front Public Health. 2017;5:258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258 |
61. | Kleiger RE, Miller JP, Bigger JT, Jr., Moss AJ. Decreased heart rate variability and its association with increased mortality after acute myocardial infarction. Am J Cardiol. 1987;59:256–62. doi: 10.1016/0002-9149(87)90795-8 |
62. | Moher D, Hopewell S, Schulz KF, Montori V, Gotzsche PC, Devereaux PJ, et al. CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. J Clin Epidemiol. 2010;63:e1–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.03.004 |
63. | Hagg D, Brenner P, Reutfors J, Li G, DiBernardo A, Boden R, et al. A register-based approach to identifying treatment-resistant depression – comparison with clinical definitions. PLoS One. 2020;15:e0236434. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236434 |
64. | McAllister-Williams RH, Arango C, Blier P, Demyttenaere K, Falkai P, Gorwood P, et al. The identification, assessment and management of difficult-to-treat depression: an international consensus statement. J Affect Disord. 2020;267:264–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.02.023 |